|
Logo

Understanding Second Language Learning Through a Sociolinguistic Lens: A Comparative Analysis of the Variationist and Identity Approaches

1 Leave a comment on paragraph 1 0 Abstract

2 Leave a comment on paragraph 2 0 The past two decades have witnessed the growing momentum of sociolinguistic theory in second language acquisition (SLA) research. Contrary to cognitive approaches that focus on modelling language structures in the learner’s mind, sociolinguistic approaches prioritise language use in context and seek to understand how social factors affect second language (L2) learning. This review aims to discuss the contributions of sociolinguistic theory to our understanding of L2 learning through a comparative analysis of two studies, which exemplify two major sociolinguistic approaches to SLA respectively: the variationist approach and the identity approach. Although both approaches address the social dimensions of L2 learning with a particular focus on language and identity, they represent two directions of theorising and methodology within the sociolinguistic model. The variationist study by Hoffman and Walker (2010) is a quantitative enquiry into the effect of ethnic identity on L2 variation, while the identity research by Chen (2010) is an ethnographic case study of how power relations affect an L2 learner’s identities and learning opportunities. Through a comparison of how these two studies conceptualise language, identity, and learner agency, this review argues that sociolinguistic perspectives shed light on the social dimensions of learner language and offer rich characterisations of language learners and learning contexts. In particular, the variationist and identity approaches offer unique insights into how identity and agency affect L2 learning. This review concludes with a brief evaluation of the contributions of sociolinguistic theory to SLA research and calls for greater synthesis of cognitive and social perspectives in exploring the intricate relationships among mind, society, and L2 learning.

3 Leave a comment on paragraph 3 0  

4 Leave a comment on paragraph 4 0 Keywords: second language learning, sociolinguistic theory, identity approach, variationist approach, language and identity

5 Leave a comment on paragraph 5 0  

  1. 6 Leave a comment on paragraph 6 0
  2. Introduction

7 Leave a comment on paragraph 7 0 1.1 Background of the Discussion

8 Leave a comment on paragraph 8 0 In 1997, Firth and Wagner argued that the predominance of cognitive perspectives had led to “imbalance” in second language acquisition (SLA) research and called for “enhanced awareness” of the social aspects[1] of second language (L2) learning (p. 285). Their work triggered a heated cognitive-social debate in SLA and galvanised a growing body of research driving the “social turn” in this field (Block, 2003). One socially oriented strand that has made great strides is the sociolinguistic model. Within this model, multiple approaches have been proposed to explain L2 learning, including the variationist approach (Preston, 2000; Tarone, 1983), language socialisation theory (Duff, 1995, 2010), the identity approach (Norton, 2000, 2013), and conversation analysis (Firth, 1996; Seedhouse, 2004).

9 Leave a comment on paragraph 9 0 Contrary to traditional cognitive approaches that focus on modelling language structures in the mind, sociolinguistic approaches prioritise language use in context and the influence of social factors on the process of L2 learning. This process not only includes language acquisition and use, but also incorporates extralinguistic issues such as learning opportunities. Specifically, the sociolinguistic model seeks to address the three basic questions of SLA (Saville-Troike, 2005) from a social perspective: What do L2 learners know about language use in context? How do learners acquire this socially situated knowledge? What are the social causes that make some learners more successful than others?

10 Leave a comment on paragraph 10 0 1.2 Focus of the Essay

11 Leave a comment on paragraph 11 1 The purpose of this essay is neither to weigh in on the social-cognitive debate nor to explicate every sociolinguistic approach to SLA. Instead, this essay aims to explore the contributions of sociolinguistic theory to our understanding of L2 learning through a comparative analysis of two studies, which exemplify two major sociolinguistic perspectives on SLA respectively: the variationist approach and the identity approach. Based on the Labovian paradigm of first language (L1) variation (Labov, 1972), variationist research in SLA shows a quantitatively-oriented interest in exploring how learner language is contextually constrained. The identity approach, on the other hand, is a relatively new strand (Norton, 2000) that employs qualitative methods to examine how power relations in learning communities construct learners’ identities and affect their learning opportunities.

12 Leave a comment on paragraph 12 0 These two approaches are selected among diverse sociolinguistic strands because they represent two fundamentally different directions of theorising within the sociolinguistic model and, as a result, different research foci and methodology. Furthermore, both approaches offer lenses through which to understand the social nature of L2 learners by addressing learner identity and agency, which constitute a distinct feature compared with traditional cognitive approaches. Accordingly, two studies are selected as telling cases of each approach. The variationist study by Hoffman and Walker (2010) is a quantitative enquiry into the effect of ethnic identity on L2 variation. In the study following the identity approach, Chen (2010) used qualitative methods to examine how power relations affect a learner’s identities and engagement in learning activities.

13 Leave a comment on paragraph 13 0 Through a comparison of how these two studies conceptualise language, identity, and learner agency, this essay argues that sociolinguistic perspectives shed light on the social dimensions of learner language and offer rich characterisations of language learners and learning contexts. In particular, the variationist and identity approaches offer unique insights into how identity and agency affect L2 learning.

14 Leave a comment on paragraph 14 0 In what follows, I first summarise the two studies. I then discuss how each approach establishes links between the social world and L2 learning through analysing the major themes emerging from the studies. This essay concludes with a brief evaluation of the contributions of sociolinguistic theory and suggestions for future SLA research.

15 Leave a comment on paragraph 15 0       2. Overview of the Two Studies

16 Leave a comment on paragraph 16 0 2.1 Ethnic Orientation and L2 Variation: A Variationist Approach to SLA

17 Leave a comment on paragraph 17 0 Situated in the multiethnic context of Toronto, Hoffman and Walker (2010) examine how ethnic identity affects English variation among Chinese and Italian residents. The two variants analysed in their study were (t/d)-deletion in word-final consonant clusters and the Canadian vowel shift. The 60 participants were stratified by ethnic origin (Chinese or Italian), immigrant generation (first or second/third), and sex (male or female). Twenty English speakers with British/Irish origins constituted a comparison group. Apart from using sociolinguistic interviews to elicit linguistic data, the authors implemented a questionnaire to quantify ethnic orientation (EO). The key findings are: (a) Degree of EO varied across and within ethnic groups; (b) patterns of variation, as indicated by rates of use, were associated with ethnic origin and degree of EO; (c) while the second/third generations resembled the British/Irish group in terms of linguistic conditioning, first-generation participants did not show such tendency. The authors conclude that language transfer exists in the first generation but does not persist in subsequent generations, and that ethnic identity plays a role in L2 variation. The authors further infer that ethno-linguistic variation “has less to do with imperfect acquisition of the majority language and more to do with the way in which speakers actively construct and express ethnic identity” (p. 59).

18 Leave a comment on paragraph 18 0 2.2 Power Relations and L2 Communities: An Identity Approach to SLA

19 Leave a comment on paragraph 19 0 Situated in an American school context, Chen’s (2010) ethnographic case study investigates identity construction of an L2 learner in three communities. The focal student, 9-year-old Evan, was an English-as-a-new-language (ENL) student who just arrived in America. The research lasted a year and gleaned rich qualitative data from interviews, observation, and student journals. The data were analysed in three steps. First, Chen recognised communities of practice in which Evan participated: the mainstream classroom, the advanced math group, and the English language learning community. Second, Chen identified characteristics of these communities, featuring institutional, instructional, discursive practices, and power relations. Third, the author identified Evan’s identities corresponding to each community. The data indicate that (a) the unbalanced power relations in three communities shaped contradictory identities for the learner; (b) the constructed identities affected the learner’s engagement in learning to varying degrees. The study suggests that ENL students may encounter identity conflicts across different school communities, and that collaborative power relations are vital for building a supportive learning environment and maximising learning opportunities.

  1. 20 Leave a comment on paragraph 20 0
  2. Social World and L2 Learning: Establishing a Link

21 Leave a comment on paragraph 21 0 3.1 Language and Identity

22 Leave a comment on paragraph 22 0 3.1.1 The view of language.

23 Leave a comment on paragraph 23 0 One assumption underpinning Hoffman and Walker’s (2010) study, and arguably most variationist-SLA research, is that learner language is a system in its own right (Selinker, 1972), and this system is inherently variable and malleable (Larsen-Freeman, 2012; Tarone, 2007). Some argue that such variability is merely evidence of developmental stages governed by Universal Grammar[2] and L1 system (Adjemian, 1976). The main problem with this argument lies in its underestimation of the role of social context. In fact, research shows that social factors can sometimes “override” the abovementioned internal factors (Tarone & Liu, 1995, p. 123) to influence L2 variation and shape L2 knowledge.

24 Leave a comment on paragraph 24 0 In Hoffman and Walker’s (2010) study, L2 variation is related to ethnic identity. Take (t/d)-deletion as an example. Table 1 displays a multivariate analysis of social factors. The statistics confirm the difference in rate of deletion between the Chinese group and other groups, suggesting that Chinese participants favoured deletion whereas the Italian and British/Irish groups disfavoured it. Moreover, within the Chinese group, the speakers with higher EO status[3] were more likely to delete (t/d), with the first-generation Chinese (.77) followed by younger Chinese participants (.56 and .55 respectively).

Table 1
Social Factors Contributing to (t/d)-Deletion

25 Leave a comment on paragraph 25 0

26 Leave a comment on paragraph 26 0 Source: Hoffman & Walker, 2010, p. 51, Table 3

27 Leave a comment on paragraph 27 0 To further verify language patterns, the authors conducted multivariate analyses of linguistic factor groups (see Table 2). The results show that for the British/Irish, the phonological effects were much stronger (range=45 and 48, compared with 12 for morphological status). In contrast, for the first-generation Chinese, the preceding phonological context was not selected as significant and the effects of the morphological status were stronger. Thus, the first-generation Chinese did not have similar linguistic conditioning with the British/Irish. However, analysed in the same way, second/third-generation Chinese participants resembled the British/Irish in linguistic conditioning to a large degree. This suggests that language transfer existed in the first generation but did not persist in younger generations. Therefore, it can be argued that variable (t/d)-deletion cannot be traced to incomplete acquisition or language transfer straightforwardly, which makes ethnic identity a plausible explanation for variation across and within ethnic groups. This study thus strengthens the case for the role of social factors in L2 variation.

Table 2
Linguistic Factors Contributing to (t/d)-Deletion

28 Leave a comment on paragraph 28 0

29 Leave a comment on paragraph 29 0 Source: Hoffman & Walker, 2010, p. 52, Table 4

30 Leave a comment on paragraph 30 0 The view that learner language is variable and socially conditioned contrasts starkly with the generativist view of linguistic knowledge as categorical (Chomsky, 1965). Chomskyan researchers in SLA assert that variation in response to social factors is part of performance, which is separate from the underlying linguistic knowledge (i.e., competence) (Gregg, 1990; Long, 1998). In response to this claim, variationists in SLA present a strong counterargument that the L2 learner’s mind is not impervious to social context; instead, it “processes L2 data differently in response to different social variables” (Tarone, 2007, p. 839). Numerous empirical studies have shown that the variable acquisition and production of L2 is related to a range of social factors, including the learning context (Charkova & Halliday, 2011; Tarone & Liu, 1995), characteristics of interlocutors (Li, 2010; Young, 1991), and characteristics of learners (Drummond, 2012; Sharma, 2005).

31 Leave a comment on paragraph 31 0 This counterargument is further strengthened by Preston’s (1996, 2000) psycholinguistic model of L2 variation. In this model, the cognitive mechanism of L2 variation is compared to the toss of a weighted coin, with each side representing a variant of a two-way linguistic variable. The likelihood of landing on one side over another depends on a confluence of social (Level I), linguistic (Level II), and temporal factors (Level III). Preston’s model implies that grammatical representations in the mind have variable strengths reflective of variable traces of three levels of factors. According to this model, we can speculate that the participants in Hoffman and Walker’s (2010) study had variable L2 knowledge in the sense that the linguistic forms they knew were not categorical or homogeneous; instead, their mental representations were affected by the variable strengths of social (e.g., ethnic identity), linguistic (e.g., the accompanying phonological context), and temporal factors (e.g., differences in relative time of acquisition across generations). This variationist model further blurs the boundary between competence and performance, and shows a potential link between cognitive and social approaches to understanding learner language.

32 Leave a comment on paragraph 32 0 The variationist view of language has important implications for L2 learning. First, L2 learning is variable in its outcome not only in the sense that learners vary in their degree of approximation to native-speaker (NS) patterns, but also that their L2 knowledge is unique and retains different traces of social meanings. This interpretation challenges the view that learner speech is mere deviation from NS speech. If we define success as NS patterns, then learners and educators should make efforts to create the social context that increases the likelihood of NS production. However, such attempts are not always realistic; even the abovementioned second/third-generation Chinese growing up in Toronto may exhibit different linguistic profiles from NSs due to uncontrollable social factors. Therefore, learners and educators should consider local learning contexts and (re)define the norms and targets against which they measure learning progress, assess proficiency level, and decide pedagogical and learning methods.

33 Leave a comment on paragraph 33 0 Second, L2 learning is more than internalisation of decontextualised grammar. The linguistic forms that learners acquire and use are fraught with social meanings. Thus, it is important for learners to develop sociolinguistic competence (Canale & Swain, 1980), that is, to actively manipulate and interpret language to fulfil social purposes, with a focus on the appropriateness of the speech in context as opposed to grammaticality in isolation (van Compernolle & Williams, 2012). For this reason, learners and educators should focus on both form and meaning to foster the ability to communicate effectively.

34 Leave a comment on paragraph 34 0 Nevertheless, Hoffman and Walker’s (2010) study also reveals several limitations of variationist research. First, variability could be attributed to unpredictable factors and the validity of any cross-sectional account of learner language is limited to a degree. As noted earlier, a host of variables ranging from the speech context to interlocutors may bear on L2 production. Thus, the ethnicity or the speech style of the interviewers in Hoffman and Walker’s (2010) research might have influenced the speech produced by the participants. A change of speech context may trigger a change in linguistic profiles. Therefore, it should be acknowledged that variationists can only get plausible close-ups of learner language rather than a panorama.

35 Leave a comment on paragraph 35 0 The second problem concerns limited accounts of language transfer. Hoffman and Walker (2010) tried to avoid this problem by using the multi-generational method to “establish a baseline for features transferred from the respective minority languages” (p. 45). By comparing linguistic conditioning across generations, the authors suggest that language transfer did not persist, which appears to consolidate the role of ethnic identity. However, the “baseline” also reflects the participants’ proficiency of the minority language[4], which may significantly affect ethnic identity. This limitation is also manifested in Preston’s model (2000), which fails to explain the interaction among three levels of factors. If language transfer is indicated by linguistic conditioning (Level II), to what extent does the effect of Levels I and III hinge on that of Level II? This question necessitates further exploration of the cognitive underpinnings of L2 variation among variationists and psychologists.

36 Leave a comment on paragraph 36 0 While variationists focus on linguistic forms of learner language, identity researchers like Chen (2010) are concerned with power relations underlying L2 learning process. Informed by poststructuralist theories, they conceptualise language as a value-laden system functioning as symbolic capital (Bourdieu, 1977). While structuralists theorise language as signs with idealised meanings signified by linguistic communities (Saussure, 1966), poststructuralists contend that language is not a set of neutral signs but situated practices in which speakers with different power and beliefs struggle to create meanings (Bakhtin, 1981). According to Bourdieu (1977), language is a form of symbolic capital that “can receive different values depending on the market on which it is offered” (p. 651). From this perspective, L2 learning is a process of negotiating access to and ownership of one’s potential and/or existing linguistic resources.

37 Leave a comment on paragraph 37 0 These views shift our focus to L2 learning situated in communities. As indicated by Chen’s (2010) study, the value assigned to learners’ L1 and L2 influences their capital and participation in learning activities. Evan’s limited English proficiency was considered as lack of significant capital in his English-dominant school. Evan’s L1, in contrast, was treated differently in different communities. For instance, in the English language learning (ELL) class, the teacher invalidated Evan’s L1 (Chinese) by explicitly stating in class that students’ native languages should only be used at home (p. 173). In one scenario, Evan wanted to write Chinese characters on the blackboard. The teacher stopped him immediately and Evan “became upset and hid himself underneath the table” (p. 174). Conversely, Evan’s L1 proficiency was valued in other classes, where the teachers allowed him to do tasks in Chinese and then translate them into English with help. A teacher also “gave Evan opportunities to teach the class some words in Chinese” (p. 169). The interview and observation data suggest that Evan was more engaged in those classes, where he had relatively more symbolic capital to negotiate participation than in the ELL class. Seen in this light, educators should “treat the linguistic and cultural capital of learners as affordances” (Darvin & Norton, 2015, p. 45) that enable learners, especially L2 beginners, to fully participate in learning communities.

38 Leave a comment on paragraph 38 0 The uneven distribution of value is manifested not only in languages, but also in language speakers. Thus, local power relations, as indicated by the differing possession of capital among community members, may affect learners’ “right to speak” (Norton, 2013, p. 48). This is exemplified by the power differentials between Evan and his peers. Evan often got into fights with his English-speaking peers because “he doesn’t understand what others are talking about”(Chen, 2010, p. 168) and some students teased him for that or for his being “a Chinese boy” (p. 172). Due to unequal power relations, what Evan lacked was not only L2 knowledge but also the legitimacy to speak. This echoes Bourdieu’s (1977) argument that linguistic competence should include “the power to impose reception” (p. 648), since speakers are not always deemed as valuable. However, some teachers addressed this problem by legitimising Evan’s other symbolic capital and establishing collaborative power relations in class. For example, they acknowledged the value of Evan’s L1 and his creativity, fostering a belief among students that Evan is a valuable community member. Under these circumstances, Evan seemed to be more engaged in academic and social activities in class.

39 Leave a comment on paragraph 39 0 As illustrated above, identity researchers focus on learning opportunities and educational equality. It comes as no surprise that scholars have challenged the relevance of such research to SLA (Gass, 1998). In fact, identity research is in line with other approaches that stress the importance of social interaction. For instance, the interactionist model (see Mackey, Abbuhl, & Gass, 2012) demonstrates that input, interaction, and output are essential for L2 acquisition. Moreover, sociocultural theorists (see Lantolf, 2000) maintain that learners’ L2 develops through scaffolding by other L2 speakers during interaction. Yet like physicists’ frictionless plane, the assumptions underlying these theories are that L2 learning communities are without conflicts and abundant opportunities for interaction are easily available. Admittedly, identity research may not explain what happens in the mind and its accounts of the effect of social factors are not as straightforward as those of variationist research. However, its relevance and unique contribution lies in its acknowledgement that L2 learning happens in communities where power relations affect one’s access to and participation in learning activities, which are related to the quantity and quality of interaction. Therefore, identity research is of particular value to educators and policy-makers, who can influence the institutional, instructional, and discursive practices that shape the power relations in learning communities.

40 Leave a comment on paragraph 40 0 To conclude, the variationist approach and the identity approach conceptualise language from completely different perspectives, the former highlighting the linguistic aspects of L2 learning whereas the latter addressing the ideological and social issues in L2 communities. While variationists can make predictive statements about how social factors influence the direction and outcome of L2 learning, identity researchers aim to identify the power dynamics that affect learners’ access to L2 resources and interactional opportunities. However, both approaches argue for a flexible and contextualised definition of language, the shapes, meanings, and value of which are variable and socially conditioned. This theoretical orientation is compatible with the complex social nature of L2 learners, a topic to which we now turn.

41 Leave a comment on paragraph 41 0 3.1.2 The view of identity.

42 Leave a comment on paragraph 42 0 Identity is generally understood as senses of self that reflect one’s position in and relationship to the world (Duff, 2012). Although both studies acknowledge that identity and language are interrelated, they diverge on the view of identity and its role in L2 learning. Such divergence results in different research methods and contributions.

43 Leave a comment on paragraph 43 0 Not only satisfied with describing L2 variation, variationist-SLA research is also interested in characterising L2 learners. Like most variationists, Hoffman and Walker (2010) view identity as relatively static membership in existing social groups and share a quantitative interest in measuring this membership. However, there are disagreements on how to operationalise identity among variationists in SLA, which mainly correspond to the first two waves of variationist sociolinguistics (Eckert, 2012).

44 Leave a comment on paragraph 44 0 The first-wave research (Labov, 1972; Trudgill, 1972) regards identity as membership in predetermined social categories, such as sex and ethnicity (Drummond & Schleef, 2016). Such research is common in SLA because the clear identification of learner groups allows researchers to easily establish correlations between linguistic and social variables, which may in turn illuminate individual differences in learning outcomes (e.g., Adamson & Regan, 1991). Yet such correlations may not always be valid, since the essentialist assumption of clear boundaries between and homogeneity within groups (Bucholtz, 2003) could mask more subtle factors that affect L2 learning. Addressing ethnic identity specifically, Hoffman and Walker (2010) note that the traditional treatment of ethnicity presumes that all group members have the same attitudes and ethnic orientation (EO), yet in reality EO may vary from individual to individual (p. 40).

45 Leave a comment on paragraph 45 0 Going beyond a monolithic view of identity, the second wave draws on social psychology and ethnography to explore participant-defined groupings that are meaningful to the speakers themselves (Drummond & Schleef, 2016). In Hoffman and Walker’s (2010) research, identity is operationalised as the perceived degree of EO. To obtain comparable data, the authors administered an EO questionnaire that includes 35 open-ended questions and assigned a score between 1 and 3[5] to each response. Table 3 shows the average mean EO index scores of each relevant group[6]. It is notable that salient differences exist between and within groups. This relates back to the linguistic patterns discussed earlier (see Tables 1 and 2), which suggest the role of EO in L2 variation, a more profound connection that lies beneath ethnic differences.

46 Leave a comment on paragraph 46 0  

Table 3Mean Ethnic Orientation Index Scores

47 Leave a comment on paragraph 47 0

48 Leave a comment on paragraph 48 0 Source: Hoffman & Walker, 2010, p. 47, Table 2

49 Leave a comment on paragraph 49 0 More importantly, an emic-oriented definition of identity casts light on the rationale behind L2 variation. For example, Hoffman and Walker (2010) conducted exploratory factor analysis to reduce questions in the questionnaire to four factors: social network, the participants’ grandparents, perceived discrimination in work, and housing. Further analysis shows that only one factor correlates significantly with the EO mean score: social network (r = .794, p<.001). The correlation indicates that high-EO participants had stronger ethnic social network, including using the minority language more often, actively participating in community activities, and having more interaction with members of their ethnic groups (p. 48). The authors further speculate that differences in social network between two ethnic groups can be attributed to their timelines of settlement (i.e. Italian immigration began much earlier than Chinese immigration) and visibility of minority status (i.e. physical or racial characteristics may affect assimilation into Caucasian communities), which conspire to facilitate or impede linguistic assimilation to local English communities (p. 56-57). These findings offer a potential rationale that goes beyond ethnic and L1 origins: integration into L2 social network may affect L2 acquisition and use, hence strengthening the argument that social factors play a role in L2 learning.

50 Leave a comment on paragraph 50 0 As exemplified by Hoffman and Walker’s (2010) study, second-wave variationist-SLA research starts to recognise the subjectivity and multiplicity of learner identity. Identity is no longer considered merely as a factor, but a lens through which to understand learners’ variable social networks. We can further speculate that the speech community to which L2 learners are connected makes available a linguistic repertoire that influences the input-interaction-output process (Mackey et al., 2012) of L2 learning. Since identities and social networks are different for L2 learners, we cannot assume that learners will acquire the full range of linguistic repertoires shared by NSs, nor can we expect that all learners will identify with NS communities. This relates back to the previously discussed question of norms and targets, which should be defined in ways that are meaningful to L2 learners themselves and reflective of local learning contexts/communities (see Geeslin & Long, 2014).

51 Leave a comment on paragraph 51 0 While variationists conceptualise identity as membership in communities and focus on its effect on learners’ L2 knowledge, identity researchers consider identity as situated position in a given context and examine its influence on learning possibilities. Like most identity researchers, Chen (2010) adopts Norton’s (2000)  poststructuralist definition of identity—“how a person understands his or her relationship to the world, how that relationship is constructed across time and space…with reference to larger, and more frequently inequitable, social structures which are reproduced in day-to-day interaction” (p. 5). In this view, identity is fluid, multiple, and context-dependent rather than fixed, unitary, and coherent (Norton & Toohey, 2011). Unlike variationists’ static view of identity as something learners have, poststructuralist theory reconceptualises identity as something learners do. In other words, learners do not always have unproblematic access to certain community and its L2 resources; instead, their identities are repeatedly negotiated through social interaction. This view of identity as dynamic necessitates qualitative methodology with a longitudinal perspective.

52 Leave a comment on paragraph 52 0 In her case study, Chen (2010) examines aspects of Evan’s identities as constructed by institutional, instructional, and discursive practices[7] in three communities. Chen’s findings of Evan’s identities are summarised in Table 4. Evan’s learning experiences varied as his identities changed across communities. For example, in the mainstream classroom and the math group, the teachers shaped a collaborative power relation in class through valuing Evan’s L1 and applauding his creativity in academic work. Under these circumstances, despite language difficulties, Evan was actively engaged in cognitively demanding tasks with appropriate scaffolding from peers and teachers (p. 170-173). In contrast, in the ELL class, the teacher authorised English proficiency only and stressed discipline in class. Situated in this context, Evan was mainly positioned as a mischievous student with language need. Consequently, he showed little interest in designated tasks and displayed disruptive behaviours. In one scenario, Evan refused to do reading exercise and hid underneath the table. When the teacher came to him, Evan went to sit on the teacher’s seat and announced that he was the teacher (p. 174). As mentioned earlier, Evan also got into fights with his peers; yet he suggested in interviews that it was not his intention to make trouble: “It’s not I want to. It is someone makes me [the troublemaker]” (p. 171). This statement indicates that although unaware of the underlying power relations at work, Evan felt explicitly that his identity was constructed by others.

Table 4
Summary of Evan’s Identities in Three Communities  

53 Leave a comment on paragraph 53 0

54 Leave a comment on paragraph 54 0 The poststructuralist view of identity casts new light on L2 learning. First, L2 learners’ commitment to learning is constructed by power relations of given contexts. The fluid nature of identity problematises the conventional view that L2 learners can be defined in binary terms as motivated/extroverted/uninhibited or unmotivated/introverted/inhibited (Norton, 2013). Indeed, the analysis of affective factors (Krashen, 1982) (e.g., motivation, self-confidence) should incorporate consideration of “What are learners allowed to do?”, which, in turn, requires an examination of power relations that structure the conditions for the realisation of learners’ desires and goals (Norton, 2013). Therefore, researchers and educators should view L2 learners as complex social beings, “whose history, dispositions toward learning, access to sociocultural worlds, participation, and imaginations together shape the quality of their achievement” (Kinginger, 2004, p. 241).

55 Leave a comment on paragraph 55 0 Second, L2 learning is a process of negotiating participation and membership in L2-related communities (Lave & Wenger, 1991). Studies have shown that access to L2 communities is not guaranteed (e.g., Giroir, 2014; Morita, 2004). Some learners may find that opportunities for L2 learning (e.g., interaction with community members) are constrained by their identities imposed by others. These identities may be related to the ideological structures and practices that are often rendered invisible in communities, such as “linguicism, racism, sexism, ageism, and/or classism” (The Douglas Fir Group, 2016, p. 32). Therefore, educators and policy-makers should develop a critical awareness of local power relations and promote equality through institutional and instructional practices (e.g., teaching materials, curriculum design, school policies, etc.).

56 Leave a comment on paragraph 56 0 Despite the abovementioned insights, Chen’s (2010) study reveals two major drawbacks of identity research. First, the link between power relations and L2 development is weak, since participation does not necessarily lead to learning (Leung, 2010). Chen (2010) fails to capture Evan’s English development (whatever kind that is) at different stages of observation in response to different power relations. This may leave unresolved questions concerning to what extent participation and commitment is translated into actual learning. Future research should provide more evidence to illustrate the impact of power on the rate, direction, or outcome of L2 learning. Second, the view of identity as multiple, fluid, and changing may lead to excessive disorganised data and subjective interpretation that undermine the validity and reliability of research. Admittedly, this methodological concern is a logical repercussion of poststructuralist theory of identity. One possible solution is to provide sufficient explanations for research process to increase transparency and reduce ethical concerns.

57 Leave a comment on paragraph 57 0 In sum, variationists and identity researchers capture different levels of learner identity, the former stressing macro-sociological categories and local social networks while the latter addressing contextually-situated positions constructed by ideologically-informed interactions. Further, they theorise the role of identity in L2 learning in different ways. While variationists seek patterns in learners’ social groupings to match L2 variation, identity researchers are interested in the contradictions manifested in individual learners’ identities, with a view to examining how power relations affect learners’ learning opportunities. Despite different research foci, both approaches tend to argue against monolithic views of identity and display emic sensitivity by incorporating learners’ accounts of social reality.

58 Leave a comment on paragraph 58 0 3.2 Learner Agency

59 Leave a comment on paragraph 59 0 The previous sections have illustrated how the social world affects L2 learners and their L2 learning. Yet learners are not passive recipients of social influences; instead, they are active agents who “can make conscious choices, resist or comply, choose learning targets, (re)shape their learning environment and learning opportunities” (Duff, 2012, p. 413). Thus, learner agency reflects learners’ own contributions to their L2 learning.

60 Leave a comment on paragraph 60 0 Both studies examined in this essay address the agentive nature of L2 learners to a degree. Hoffman and Walker (2010) speculate that linguistic features are associated with “particular social distinctions and values”, based on which learners “adopt and use these features strategically” to express ethnic identities (p. 59). Seen in this light, learners can not only choose target-language models they prefer (Beebe, 1985), but also create their own patterns of L2 with meanings and uses related to their needs and contexts,  rather than conform to the L2 system imposed by others (Canagarajah, 2016; Larsen-Freeman & Cameron, 2008). Deviation from NS-variety, therefore, may not be errors, but a legitimate language choice. Although Hoffman and Walker (2010) did not directly investigate learner agency, their speculation represents a recent trend in variationist-SLA research, which addresses conscious language choice made by learners, with a particular focus on learners’ attitudes toward L2 varieties (e.g., Howard, 2012; Rindal, 2010; Rindal & Piercy, 2013).

61 Leave a comment on paragraph 61 0 However, such research faces certain challenges. First, exercising agency to express identities requires sophisticated sociolinguistic competence, which is primarily limited to advanced L2 learners. Second, agency “does not straightforwardly lead to empowerment” (Miller, 2010, p. 485), since agents are influenced by social structures. This can be related to identity researchers’ view of language as symbolic capital. In a linguistic marketplace, different languages and varieties are assigned different values. Therefore, learners’ language choice is conditioned by power relations to varying degrees.

62 Leave a comment on paragraph 62 0 In Chen’s (2010) study, agency is conceptualised as learners’ investment (Norton, 2000) in L2 learning. If learners invest in a language or literacy practices, “they do so with the understanding that they will acquire a wider range of symbolic and material resources, which will in turn increase the value of their cultural capital and social power” (Darvin & Norton, 2015, p. 37). For example, Evan invested in learning activities in the mainstream classroom not only because the power relations constructed favourable identities for him, but also because he wanted to retain these identities and gain more social (e.g., better relationships with peers) and symbolic capital (e.g., English proficiency) in the communities. Hence, L2 learners’ investment in learning should also be viewed as “investment in their own identities” (Norton, 2013, p. 11). However, it should be noted that Chen (2010) characterised Evan’s investment mainly in terms of his active participation in learning communities and neglected the fact that one can express agency by deliberately not acting (van Lier, 2008). For example, silence and non-participation may also represent investment for particular purposes.

63 Leave a comment on paragraph 63 0 To conclude, learner agency is still a relatively new topic in SLA that requires further theorising and empirical evidence (Deters, Gao, Vitanova, & Miller, 2014). Variationists and identity researchers share the belief that learner agency, whether language choice or investment, may dramatically affect L2 learning trajectories and outcomes (De Costa & Norton, 2016; Duff & Doherty, 2015; Nance, McLeod, O’Rourke, & Dunmore, 2016). Future research could examine “the structure and agency dilemma” (Block, 2013, p. 126) and focus on how learners can harness the affordances available in the learning context (Deters et al., 2014).

  1. 64 Leave a comment on paragraph 64 0
  2. Conclusion

65 Leave a comment on paragraph 65 0 The rise of the sociolinguistic model has advanced our understanding of L2 learning by reconceptualising the nature of learner language and identity and emphasising the role of learner agency. Unlike traditional cognitive approaches that consider language as isolated neutral structures to be internalised in the mind and L2 learners as language processors, sociolinguistic theory foregrounds the social nature of language and views L2 learners as social beings situated in communities. It also challenges the conventional cognitivist view of L2 learners as passive recipients of language by highlighting learner agency.

66 Leave a comment on paragraph 66 0 This essay has discussed the contributions of sociolinguistic theory by focusing on two theoretical strands: the variationist approach and the identity approach. Although both approaches address the social dimensions of L2 learning, they represent two directions of theorising and methodology within the sociolinguistic model. The quantitatively-oriented variationist approach seeks to establish correlations between social and linguistic variables, with a view to describing and explaining learner language. It also explores learner identity through seeking patterns of social groupings, which are expected to match L2 variation. The identity approach, on the other hand, employs qualitative methods to uncover the ideological underpinnings of L2 learning, with a focus on the effect of power relations on individual learners’ identities and learning opportunities. Moreover, both approaches show an increasing interest in agency, with variationists suggesting learners’ language choice and identity researchers highlighting learners’ investment in language in return for more symbolic and/or material benefits.

67 Leave a comment on paragraph 67 0 As illustrated by these two approaches, the sociolinguistic model has contributed to the expansion of “the ontological and empirical parameters of SLA” (Firth & Wagner, 1997, p. 295), transforming the field from an acquisition-centric and cognitive-dominant paradigm to a multi-layered ecosystem in which cognitive and social dimensions of L2 learning are interrelated (see The Douglas Fir Group, 2016). As suggested in my discussion of the compatibility of sociolinguistic theory with other approaches in SLA, the cognitive and social perspectives may not be divided in the sense that they are theoretically mutually exclusive. Instead, their tension may derive from the fact that each approach addresses different levels of L2 learning (e.g., from the level of brain to the level of society).

68 Leave a comment on paragraph 68 0 However, as indicated by the limitations of the two approaches discussed in this essay, social approaches need to further draw on cognitive perspectives to better answer the question of how learners acquire socially-situated L2 knowledge. Moreover, for the social strands that prioritise contexts and interactions, one of the most urgent tasks is to establish stronger links between social participation and learning outcomes, so that we can identify the social causes that directly affect L2 development.

69 Leave a comment on paragraph 69 0 To build on what has been achieved since “the social turn” (Block, 2003) and become a more robust enterprise, SLA research should move toward greater integration of different levels of L2 learning and take on board the need for greater conceptual flexibility and interdisciplinarity. Some scholars in SLA have pointed out the possibilities of social-cognitive synthesis. For instance, Aktinson (2011) suggests a sociocognitive model that explains how “mind, body, and world work together” in L2 learning (p. 143). Larsen-Freeman (2011) proposes complexity theory that views language as “a complex adaptive system” influenced by cognitive and social factors (p. 49). SLA research could expect worthwhile returns as increased investment is made in a greater synthesis of cognitive and social perspectives.

70 Leave a comment on paragraph 70 0

71 Leave a comment on paragraph 71 0 References

72 Leave a comment on paragraph 72 0 Adamson, H. D., & Regan, V. M. (1991). The acquisition of community speech norms by Asian immigrants learning English as a second language: A preliminary study. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 13(1), 1–22.

73 Leave a comment on paragraph 73 0 Adjemian, C. (1976). On the nature of interlanguage systems. Language Learning, 26(2), 297–320.

74 Leave a comment on paragraph 74 0 Atkinson, D. (2011). A sociocognitive approach to second language acquisition. In D. Atkinson (Ed.), Alternative approaches to second language acquisition (pp. 143-166). Abingdon and New York: Routledge.

75 Leave a comment on paragraph 75 0 Bakhtin, M. (1981). The dialogic imagination. Austin: University of Texas Press.

76 Leave a comment on paragraph 76 0 Beebe, L. (1985). Input: Choosing the right stuff. In S. Gass & C. Madden (Eds.), Input in second language acquisition (pp. 404-414). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.

77 Leave a comment on paragraph 77 0 Block, D. (2003). The social turn in second language acquisition. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press

78 Leave a comment on paragraph 78 0 Block, D. (2013). The structure and agency dilemma in identity and intercultural communication research. Language and Intercultural Communication, 13(2), 126–147.

79 Leave a comment on paragraph 79 0 Bourdieu, P. (1977). The economics of linguistic exchanges. Social Science Information, 16(6), 645–668.

80 Leave a comment on paragraph 80 0 Bucholtz, M. (2003). Sociolinguistic nostalgia and the authentication of identity. Journal of Sociolinguistics, 7(3), 398–416.

81 Leave a comment on paragraph 81 0 Canagarajah, S. (2016). Crossing borders, addressing diversity. Language Teaching, 49(3), 438–454.

82 Leave a comment on paragraph 82 0 Canale, M., & Swain, M. (1980). Theoretical bases of communicative approaches to second language teaching and testing. Applied Linguistics, 1(1), 1–47.

83 Leave a comment on paragraph 83 0 Charkova, K. D., & Halliday, L. J. (2011). Second- and foreign-language variation in tense backshifting in indirect reported speech. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 33(1), 1–32.

84 Leave a comment on paragraph 84 0 Chen, X. (2010). Identity construction and negotiation within and across school communities: The case of one English-as-a-new-language (ENL) student. Journal of Language, Identity, and Education, 9(3), 163–179.

85 Leave a comment on paragraph 85 0 Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

86 Leave a comment on paragraph 86 0 Darvin, R., & Norton, B. (2015). Identity and a model of investment in applied linguistics. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 35, 36–56.

87 Leave a comment on paragraph 87 0 Deters, P., Gao, X., Vitanova, G., & Miller, E. R. (2014). Theorizing and analyzing agency in second language learning: Interdisciplinary approaches. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.

88 Leave a comment on paragraph 88 0 De Costa, P., & Norton, B. (2016). Identity in language learning and teaching: Research agendas for the future. In S. Preece (Ed.), The Routledge handbook of language and identity (pp. 596–601). Abingdon: Routledge.

89 Leave a comment on paragraph 89 0 Drummond, R. (2012). Aspects of identity in a second language: ING variation in the speech of Polish migrants living in Manchester, UK. Language Variation and Change, 24. 107-133.

90 Leave a comment on paragraph 90 0 Drummond, R. & Schleef, E. (2016). Identity in variationist sociolinguistics. In S. Preece. (Ed.), The routledge handbook of language and identity (pp. 50-65). Abingdon: Routledge.

91 Leave a comment on paragraph 91 0 Duff, P. A. (1995). An ethnography of communication in immersion classrooms in Hungary. TESOL Quarterly, 29(3), 505–537.

92 Leave a comment on paragraph 92 0 Duff, P. A. (2010). Language socialization. In N. H. Hornberger (Ed.), Sociolinguistics and language education (pp. 427-454). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.

93 Leave a comment on paragraph 93 0 Duff, P. A. (2012). Identity, agency, and second language acquisition. In S. M. Gass & A. Mackey (Eds.), The routledge handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 410-426). Abingdon: Routledge.

94 Leave a comment on paragraph 94 0 Duff, P. A., & Doherty, L. (2015). Examining agency in (second) language socialization research. In P. Deters, X. Gao, E. Miller, & G. Vitanova (Eds.), Interdisciplinary approaches to theorizing and analyzing agency and second language learning (pp. 54–72). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.

95 Leave a comment on paragraph 95 0 Eckert, P. (2012). Three waves of variation study: The emergence of meaning in the study of sociolinguistic variation. Annual Review of Anthropology, 41(1), 87–100.

96 Leave a comment on paragraph 96 0 Firth, A. (1996). The discursive accomplishment of normality: On ‘lingua franca’English and conversation analysis. Journal of Pragmatics, 26(2), 237–259.

97 Leave a comment on paragraph 97 0 Firth, A., & Wagner, J. (1997). On discourse, communication, and (some) fundamental concepts in SLA research. Modern Language Journal, 81(3), 285-300.

98 Leave a comment on paragraph 98 0 Gass, S. M. (1997). Input, interaction, and the second language learner. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

99 Leave a comment on paragraph 99 0 Gass, S. M. (1998). Apples and oranges: or, why apples are not orange and don’t need to be a response to Firth and Wagner. The Modern Language Journal, 82(1), 83–90.

100 Leave a comment on paragraph 100 0 Geeslin, K. L., & Long, A. Y. (2014). Sociolinguistics and second language acquisition: Learning to use language in context. New York: Routledge.

101 Leave a comment on paragraph 101 0 Giroir, S. (2014). Narratives of participation, identity, and positionality: Two cases of Saudi learners of English in the United States. TESOL Quarterly, 48(1), 34–56.

102 Leave a comment on paragraph 102 0 Gregg, K. R. (1990). The variable competence model of second language acquisition, and why it isn’t. Applied Linguistics, 11(4), 364–383.

103 Leave a comment on paragraph 103 0 Hoffman, M. F., & Walker, J. A. (2010). Ethnolects and the city: Ethnic orientation and linguistic variation in Toronto English. Language Variation and Change, 22(1), 37–67.

104 Leave a comment on paragraph 104 0 Howard, M. (2012). The advanced learner’s sociolinguistic profile: On issues of individual differences, second language exposure conditions, and type of sociolinguistic variable. Modern Language Journal, 96(1), 20–33.

105 Leave a comment on paragraph 105 0 Kinginger, C. (2004). Alice doesn’t live here anymore: Foreign language learning and identity reconstruction. In A. Pavlenko & A. Blackledge (Eds.), Negotiation of identities in multilingual contexts (pp. 219-242). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.

106 Leave a comment on paragraph 106 0 Krashen, S. D. (1982). Principles and practice in second language acquisition. Oxford: Pergamon.

107 Leave a comment on paragraph 107 0 Labov, W. (1972). Sociolinguistic Patterns. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.

108 Leave a comment on paragraph 108 0 Lantolf, J. P. (2000). Sociocultural theory and second language learning. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

109 Leave a comment on paragraph 109 0 Larsen-Freeman, D. (2011). A complexity theory approach to second language development/acquisition. In D. Atkinson (Ed.), Alternative approaches to second language acquisition (pp. 48-72). Abingdon and New York: Routledge.

110 Leave a comment on paragraph 110 0 Larsen-Freeman, D. (2012). Complex, dynamic systems: A new transdisciplinary theme for applied linguistics? Language Teaching, 45(2), 202–214.

111 Leave a comment on paragraph 111 0 Larsen-Freeman, D., & Cameron, L. (2008). Complex systems and applied linguistics. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

112 Leave a comment on paragraph 112 0 Lave, J., & Wenger, E. (1991). Situated learning: Legitimate peripheral participation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

113 Leave a comment on paragraph 113 0 Leung, C. (2010). English as an additional language: Learning and participating in mainstream classrooms. In P. Seedhouse, S. Walsh, & C. Jenks (Eds.), Conceptualising ‘learning’ in applied linguistics (pp. 182-205). Hampshire: Palgrave Macmillan.

114 Leave a comment on paragraph 114 0 Long, M. (1998). SLA: Breaking the siege. University of Hawai’i Working Papers in ESL, 17, 79-129.

115 Leave a comment on paragraph 115 0 Li, X. (2010). Sociolinguistic variation in the speech of learners of Chinese as a second language. Language Learning, 60(2), 366–408.

116 Leave a comment on paragraph 116 0 Mackey, A., Abbuhl, R. & Gass, S. M. (2012). Interactionist approach. In S. M. Gass & A. Mackey (Eds.), The routledge handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 7-23). Abingdon: Routledge.

117 Leave a comment on paragraph 117 0 Miller, E. R. (2010). Agency in the making: Adult immigrants’ accounts of language learning and work. TESOL Quarterly, 44(3), 465–487.

118 Leave a comment on paragraph 118 0 Morita, N. (2004). Negotiating participation and identity in second language academic communities. TESOL Quarterly, 38(4), 573–603.

119 Leave a comment on paragraph 119 0 Nance, C., McLeod, W., O’Rourke, B., & Dunmore, S. (2016). Identity, accent aim, and motivation in second language users: New Scottish Gaelic speakers’ use of phonetic variation. Journal of Sociolinguistics, 20(2), 164–191.

120 Leave a comment on paragraph 120 0 Norton, B. (2000). Identity and language learning: Gender, ethnicity and educational change. Essex: Pearson Education Limited.

121 Leave a comment on paragraph 121 0 Norton, B. (2013). Identity and language learning: Extending the conversation. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.

122 Leave a comment on paragraph 122 0 Norton, B., & Toohey, K. (2011). Identity, language learning, and social change. Language Teaching, 44(4), 412–446.

123 Leave a comment on paragraph 123 0 Preston, D. R. (1996). Variationist perspectives on second language acquisition. In R. Bayley & D. R. Preston (Eds.), Second language acquisition and linguistic variation (pp. 1–45). Amsterdam : Benjamins.

124 Leave a comment on paragraph 124 0 Preston, D. R.  (2000). Three kinds of sociolinguistics and SLA: A psycholinguistic perspective. In B. Swierzbin, F. Morris, M. Anderson, C. Klee, & E. Tarone (Eds.),   Social and cognitive factors in SLA (pp.3–30). Somerville , MA : Cascadilla Press.

125 Leave a comment on paragraph 125 0 Rindal, U. (2010). Constructing identity with L2: Pronunciation and attitudes among Norwegian learners of English. Journal of Sociolinguistics, 14(2), 240–261.

126 Leave a comment on paragraph 126 0 Rindal, U., & Piercy, C. (2013). Being ‘neutral’? English pronunciation among Norwegian learners. World Englishes, 32(2), 211–229.

127 Leave a comment on paragraph 127 0 Saussure, F. de. (1966). Course in general linguistics. (W. Baskin, Trans.). New York: McGraw-Hill.

128 Leave a comment on paragraph 128 0 Saville-Troike, M. (2005). Introducing second language acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

129 Leave a comment on paragraph 129 0 Seedhouse, P. (2004). The interactional architecture of the language classroom: A conversation analysis perspective. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.

130 Leave a comment on paragraph 130 0 Selinker, L. (1972). Interlanguage. IRAL—International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching, 10(3), 209–232.

131 Leave a comment on paragraph 131 0 Sharma, D. (2005). Dialect stabilization and speaker awareness in non-native varieties of English. Journal of Sociolinguistics, 9(2), 194–224.

132 Leave a comment on paragraph 132 0 Tarone, E. (1983). On the variability of interlanguage systems*. Applied Linguistics, 4(2), 142–164.

133 Leave a comment on paragraph 133 0 Tarone, E. (1988). Variation in interlanguage. London: Arnold.

134 Leave a comment on paragraph 134 0 Tarone, E. (2007). Sociolinguistic approaches to second language acquisition research 1997-2007. The Modern Language Journal, 91, 837–848.

135 Leave a comment on paragraph 135 0 Tarone, E., & Liu, G. (1995). Social context, variation and SLA theory. In G. Cook & B. Seidhofer (Eds.), Principle and practice in applied linguistics: Studies in honour of H. G. Widdowson (pp. 107–124). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

136 Leave a comment on paragraph 136 0 The Douglas Fir Group. (2016). A transdisciplinary framework for SLA in a multilingual world. The Modern Language Journal, 100(S1), 19–47.

137 Leave a comment on paragraph 137 0 Trudgill, P. (1972). Sex, covert prestige and linguistic change in the urban British English of Norwich. Language in Society, 1(2), 179–195.

138 Leave a comment on paragraph 138 0 van Compernolle, R. A., & Williams, L. (2012). Reconceptualizing sociolinguistic competence as mediated action: Identity, meaning-making, agency. The Modern Language Journal, 96(2), 234–250.

139 Leave a comment on paragraph 139 0 van Lier, L. (2008). Agency in the classroom. In J. Lantolf & M. Poehner (Eds.), Sociocultural theory and the teaching of second languages (pp. 163-186). London: Equinox.

140 Leave a comment on paragraph 140 0 Young, R. (1991). Variation in interlanguage morphology. New York: Peter Lang.

141 Leave a comment on paragraph 141 0  

142 Leave a comment on paragraph 142 0 [1] Social factors have figured in explaining SLA, such as the identification of interaction as a significant element in L2 learning (e.g., Gass, 1997). What was new in Firth and Wagner’s (1997) claims was a call for increased “emic sensitivity” (p. 286) and empirical focus on L2 learning situated in context, as opposed to the exclusive focus on the mental process of internalising linguistic structures.

143 Leave a comment on paragraph 143 0 [2] Universal Grammar is a theory proposed by Noam Chomsky, who posits that language is an innate ability of human beings and that there are principles that all natural human languages share.

144 Leave a comment on paragraph 144 0 [3] First-generation Chinese participants were reported to have the highest EO status, a point to which we will return.

145 Leave a comment on paragraph 145 0  

146 Leave a comment on paragraph 146 0 [4] Most second/third-generation Chinese participants were bilinguals while the first generation was minority-language dominant.

147 Leave a comment on paragraph 147 0 [5] For scalability, the researchers assigned 1 to responses that suggest least engagement in the ethnic group, 3 to maximum involvement, and 2 to intermediate or mixed responses.

148 Leave a comment on paragraph 148 0 [6] The authors used high/low EO to further categorise second/third generation. Statistical tests showed the best scaling reliability was achieved when participants were divided between those scored 1.50 or higher (high EO) and those scored lower than 1.49 (low EO) (Cronbach’s α = .905).

149 Leave a comment on paragraph 149 0 [7] Institutional practices are adopted practices to group members based on specific criteria. Instructional practices are used to organise the content and format of teaching and learning activities. Discursive practices are comments on the social, linguistic, and academic competencies of the focal student (Chen, 2010, p. 167).

Source: https://corerj.soc.srcf.net/?page_id=320&replytopara=11